May 4: Quirks goes to the dogs | CBC Radio - Action News
Home WebMail Saturday, November 23, 2024, 04:21 AM | Calgary | -12.0°C | Regions Advertise Login | Our platform is in maintenance mode. Some URLs may not be available. |
Quirks and Quarks

May 4: Quirks goes to the dogs

An entire show dedicated to some of the latest science around our favourite canine companions, from dog intelligence and longevity, how dogs can sniff out PTSD, an extinct Indigenous dog, and even a look at why some dogs will do anything for a treat.

A whole show dedicated to the latest science around our canine companions

An illustration of a white fluffy dog with a fox-like face and pointy ears.
In a recent study, researchers mapped out the full genome of a Salish Woolly Dog, an extinct breed of dog that was kept by Indigenous people along the Pacific coast for thousands of years. As a part of the study, researchers made this full-body forensic reconstruction of a woolly dog, based on a 160-year-old pelt in the Smithsonians collection, as well as archaeological remains. (Karen Carr)

Originally published on May 4, 2024


On this week's special episode of Quirks & Quarks, we dedicate a wholehour todog science:

We baby talk with both dogs and kids, but our faces say something different

While we use a similar tone of voice when talking to preverbal babies and dogs, the facial expressions that accompany the baby talk are very different. When researcher Anna Gergely filmed interactions in families that had both an infant and a dog, she saw that the parents would often widen their eyes and smile as they talked to the child, but kept their faces neutral when talking to the dog. In the paper published in Applied Animal Behaviour Science, Gergely and her colleagues say that while big eyes and a smile denote a friendly face for us, it could signal the opposite for our canine companions.

A dark-haired woman sits opposite of a brown dog
A new study describes how we use 'baby talk' with both dogs and infants, but keep our facial expressions distinct for both. (Kos-Huts et al.)

Dogs can use their powerful noses to sniff out PTSD

It's no secret that dogs have an incredibly powerful sense of smell, but it turns out that they can also sniff out our emotional state. New research from Dalhousie University has found that dogs can sense an oncoming post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) flashback, by detecting compounds in our breath. Sherry Stewart, a Professor of psychiatry and Canada Research Chair in Addictions and Mental Health at Dalhousie University, thinks this could help PTSD patients, as they can intervene much earlier than relying on our physical cues. The research was published in the journal Frontiers In Allergy.

A woman is holding the paws of a dog, looking happy.
PhD student Laura Kiiroja trained dogs to sniff out trauma responses. (Submitted by Sherry Stewart)

A quarter of all Labradors are hard-wired to be hungrier and burn less energy

Dogs that carry a genetic mutation to a gene that plays a critical role in hunger and energy may be hard-wired to gain weight. In a new study in the journal Science Advances, scientists described how this mutation slows dogs' metabolism and makes them feel hungrier. This mutation is present in a quarter of all Labrador retrievers and in 66 per cent of flat-coated retrievers. Eleanor Raffan, an assistant professor of Systems Physiology at the University of Cambridge, said owners of these dogs need to be particularly careful to prevent them becoming overweight.

A brown dog that looks like a flat-coated retriever stares longingly at a woman's sandwich.
A quarter of all Labrador retrievers and 66% of flat-coated retrievers that come in black, chocolate and kidney colours are genetically predisposed to gaining weight. (Matt Cardy/Getty Images)

Your pet dog may know more words than you give them credit for

Figuring out if dogs can understand object words like "ball" or "frisbee" can be a challenge since most don't do better than chance when they're asked to fetch objects. This is why scientists decided to look inside the dogs' brains instead. In an experiment described in the journal Current Biology, scientists found that a majority of dogs tested did seem to show brain activity indicating understanding of words for toys and other objects. Marianna Boros, a postdoctoral researcher at Etvs Lornd University in Hungary, said this suggests that dogs have a "mental representation" of certain objects that's activated when they hear the object's name.

A cute dog that looks like it might be a beagle cross sniffs at a toy rope on the ground as it looks guiltily sideways at the camera.
A new study found that dogs tend to be pretty good at understanding that we use some words to describe different objects. (Rob Carr/The Associated Press)

WEB EXCLUSIVE:Size, face shape and other factors matter when it comes to a dog's lifespan, study shows

A new study of dog breeds confirms that miniature Dachshunds may outlive an English bulldogs, and builds a better picture of life expectancy for different breeds of dogs. Researchers from the U.K.-based dog welfare charity Dogs Trust analyzed data on over half a million dogs. Their findings, which were published in Scientific Reports, suggest that a dog's average lifespan is affected by their breed, sex, body size and face shape. Dogs Trust data manager Kirsten McMillan says these results can help inform the owners' choice of future canine companions, and create better practices for breeders.

A puppy sitting on a white cushion faces the camera
Small long-nosed dogs live longer, on average, than medium-sized flat-faced pups, new U.K. study suggests. (Dogs Trust)

It's possible and worthwhile to teach an old dog new tricks

A recent study in the journal Geroscience dove deep into the thinking capabilities of domesticated dogs, and found that these decline as the dogs age but only if they're not healthy. Dora Szabo from the Senior Family Dog Project at Etvs Lornd University in Budapest was part of the team, which tested over 120 dogs over several years. They found that dogs who are kept well-trained and active tend to be smarter and are better protected from 'dog dementia.' Szabo says owners can help dogs stay sharp by keeping active and learning new skills the same preventive medicine prescribed to us humans.

A person is jogging with their dog. The dog has reddish-brown fur, and the owner is wearing a dark t-shirt and leggings and blue jogging shoes.
Dogs' cognitive skills can decline as they age, but keeping them active and healthy is a way to keep them mentally sharp, a new study shows. (Ivanoh Demers/CBC/Radio-Canada)

What a genome reveals about an extinct breed of dogs and the Indigenous people who cared for them

The woolly dog was a breed that lived alongside Indigenous Coast Salish people in the Pacific Northwest for thousands of years. One of the reasons they were kept was for their fur so thick and luxurious that it could be spun into yarn. But the dogs and the artifacts woven from their fur, like blankets, were victims of European settler authorities attempting to erase Indigenous culture, and they quickly went extinct. The only known remaining wooly dog specimen was misplaced in the archives at the Smithsonian for over a hundred years, and only recently uncovered.

Now, an international team of researchers, including paleogeneticist Audrey Lin, has fully sequenced the dog's genome and revealed the genetic secrets to its infamous fluffy hair. The team also worked with Coast Salish elders, knowledge keepers, weavers, and artists to get a full picture of the life of these cherished animals.The research was published in the journal Science.

Quirks & Quarks producer Amanda Buckiewicz spoke with:

  • Audrey Lin, a Gerstner Postdoctoral Scholar at the American Museum of Natural History, and Research Associate at the Smithsonian's National Museum of Natural History
  • Steven Point, Grand Chief of the St:l People.
  • Violet Elliott, a master weaver living in the traditional territory of the Cowichan Tribes.
A white fur pelt against a black background.
The 160-year-old pelt of the woolly dog Mutton in the Smithsonians collection. (Brittany M. Hance, Smithsonian )